Nicaraguan Sign Language

Deaf-community sign language
(Learn how and when to remove this template message)
Nicaraguan Sign Language
Idioma de Señas de Nicaragua, ISN
Native toNicaragua
RegionThe Managua region and spreading throughout the country
Native speakers
3,000 (1997)[1]
Language family
Deaf-community sign language: developed as a creole language from home and idioglossic sign, with the addition of an ASL-influenced manual alphabet
Language codes
ISO 639-3ncs
Glottolognica1238
ELPNicaraguan Sign Language

Nicaraguan Sign Language (ISN; Spanish: Idioma de Señas de Nicaragua) is a form of sign language developed by deaf children in a number of schools in Nicaragua in the 1980s. It is of particular interest to linguists as they view it as a unique opportunity to study what many view to be the birth of a new language.

History

Before the 1970s, a deaf community largely socializing with and amongst each other was not present in Nicaragua.[2] Deaf people were generally isolated from one another and mostly used simple home sign systems and gesture (mímicas) to communicate with their families and friends, though there were several cases of idioglossia among deaf siblings.[3] The conditions necessary for a language to arise occurred in 1977, when a center for special education established a scheme which was initially attended by 50 deaf children. The number of pupils at the school (in the Managua neighborhood of San Judas) then grew to 100 by 1979, the beginning of the Sandinista Revolution.

In 1980 a vocational school for deaf adolescents was opened in the Villa Libertad area of Managua. By 1983 more than 400 deaf pupils were enrolled in the two schools. Initially the language scheme emphasized spoken Spanish and lipreading, and the use of signs by teachers was limited to fingerspelling (using simple signs to sign the alphabet).

The scheme achieved little success, with most pupils failing to grasp the concept of Spanish words. The children subsequently remained linguistically disconnected from their teachers, but the schoolyard, the street, and the school bus provided fertile ground for them to communicate with one another. By combining gestures and elements of their home-sign systems, a pidgin-like form and a creole-like language rapidly emerged — they were creating their own language. The "first-stage" pidgin has been called Lenguaje de Signos Nicaragüense (LSN) and is still used by many who attended the school at the time.[when?]

Staff at the school, unaware of the development of this new language, saw the children's gesturing as mime and a failure to acquire Spanish. Unable to understand what the children were saying, they asked for outside help. In June 1986 the Nicaraguan Ministry of Education contacted Judy Kegl, an American Sign Language linguist from MIT. As Kegl and other researchers began to analyze the language they noticed that the young children had taken the pidgin-like form of the older children to a higher level of complexity, with verb agreement and other conventions of grammar. The more complex sign language is now known as Idioma de Señas de Nicaragua (ISN).

From the beginning of her research until Nicaraguan Sign Language was well established, Kegl carefully avoided introducing the sign languages that she knew, in particular American Sign Language, to the deaf community in Nicaragua.[4] Critics argued a form of linguistic imperialism had been occurring internationally for decades, in which individuals would introduce ASL to populations of deaf people in other countries, often supplanting existing local sign languages. Kegl's policy was to document and study rather than to impose or change the language or its community. Whilst she did not interfere with deaf Nicaraguans gaining exposure to other sign languages, she did not introduce such opportunities. She has, however, documented contact and influences with other sign languages that have occurred since the 1990s. Critics, such as Felicia Ackerman, have taken issue with the ethics of isolating the Nicaraguan children.[5]

Kegl's organization, Nicaraguan Sign Language Projects, helped establish a deaf school staffed entirely by deaf Nicaraguan teachers and has supported deaf Nicaraguans in attending and presenting at international conferences.[6]

Linguistics

ISN offers a rare opportunity to study the emergence of a new language. Before ISN, studies of the early development of languages had focused on creoles, which develop from the mixture of two (or more) distinct communities of fluent speakers. In contrast, ISN was developed by a group of young people with only non-conventional home sign systems and gesture.

Some linguists, such as Kegl[7] and R. J. Senghas,[8] view the sign language development in Managua as proof that language acquisition is hard-wired inside the human brain. Steven Pinker, author of The Language Instinct, claims that "The Nicaraguan case is absolutely unique in history ... We've been able to see how it is that children—not adults—generate language, and we have been able to record it happening in great scientific detail. And it's the only time that we've actually seen a language being created out of thin air."

Since 1990, other researchers including Ann Senghas, Marie Coppola, Richard Senghas, Laura Polich and Jennie Pyers, have begun to study and report on the development of this unique language and its community. Since then, whilst researchers have their own interpretations of the events leading to the language and its development since, all agree that to date, the phenomenon being studied is one of the richest sources of data on language emergence discovered.

Structure

Bierma[9] provides a good foundation to ISN structures. He takes as an example "rolling down". While a person may visualize this as one motion, ISN splits this action into two parts: manner and direction. These smaller parts allow for them to be rearranged to create different phrases. The most sophisticated speakers use an A-B-A speech pattern; in our example, this reflects the signs roll-down-roll, to note that the rolling down motion is continuous, not that the subject rolled and then descended.

Spatial modulation

"See" (a) and "Pay" (b), produced in a neutral direction and spatially modulated to the signer's left

Spatial modulations are the building blocks of all sign languages studied to date. A neutrally placed sign is in front of the chest; however, a sign can be modulated, or directionally altered, to convey many grammatical changes.

Spatial modulations can perform functions including "indicating person or number; providing deictic, locative, or temporal information; or indicating grammatical relationships".[10]

In the article written by Senghas and Coppola, they explore spatial modulation as it occurs in ISN. They found that this movement from the neutral space was much more common among signers who began learning at a younger age than their peers who did so when they were older. Taking this into consideration (as well as their studies on spatial modulations for indicating shared reference and the speed at which signers at different stages of learning signed), Senghas and Coppola determined that child learners are creating Nicaraguan Sign Language – they "changed the language as they learned it".[11] The fact that students who began signing at a younger age use spatial modulation more often than their older peers, who began signing at ISN's conception, is indicative that the language matures as the younger cohorts make the grammar more complex. They go on to note that it is only when a language is not matured, such as with ISN, that language-learning abilities show their transformational and creative capacity.

In the signing space, the use of pointing to indicate referent identity has increased greatly since the 1980s. Points can serve a "pronoun-like function, coordinating with the spatial modulations to verbs, to indicate the argument structure of the sentence, and to co-index referents across discourse".[12][full citation needed]

Illustration of unrotated and rotated representation of a giving event. The location is compared with the man's location in the event to determine the rotation of the representation.

Senghas and Coppola have noted that signers who learned ISN before the Extensive Contact Period (before 1983) were inconsistent in whether an event was represented as rotated or reflected (unrotated) in the signing space. If the signer was watching an event where a man on the signer's left gave an object to a woman on the signer's right, it was at random (when the signer reiterated the scene) whether or not the signer would use spatial modulation to mark left and right based on his view from in front of the scene or as if the signer were facing the same way as the actors in the scene.

Signers who began learning after 1983 were not inconsistent in that way. Across multiple signers and multiple scenes, signers would apply the same rotated representation. Senghas and Coppola suggested that this meant that "spatial modulations are being used as a shared grammatical element among this age cohort".[13]

Academic interpretation

When it became language

Researchers disagree regarding at what stage in the development of ISN it became a full-fledged language.

Evidence for innate language capacities

William Stokoe, known by many as the father of American Sign Language linguistics, disagreed that the emergence of ISN is evidence of a language acquisition device. Stokoe also questions assertions that the language has emerged entirely without outside influence from, for example, Spanish or ASL.[14] There is so far no final evidence available to resolve the controversy surrounding nativism v. cultural learning, and the dispute reaches far into theoretical linguistics, the approaches of which may conceptualize grammar in different and incompatible ways. Even if the evidence collected seems to indicate a lack of access to Spanish and ASL in the early emergence process, the possibility remains that the development of ISN is facilitated by the speaker's exposure to more general communicative strategies in early infancy.

Alternatives to theories proposing a language acquisition device have been presented by Michael Tomasello (among others).[citation needed] Tomasello argues that the process of acquiring a first language is boosted by non-linguistic communication, as in the establishment of joint intentional frames and in the understanding of communicative intentions.[15]

In any case, once ISN came into being, like other languages it actively engaged in contact with languages in its environment.

As "unwritable"

R. J. Senghas (1997) used the phrase "unspeakable, unwritable" language in the title of his dissertation to highlight the common misconception that those languages without a written form are not as "real" (a view often held by those who do not study indigenous languages). In a similar fashion, sign languages are often not given proper recognition because they are not spoken or written. (Senghas has never claimed that Nicaraguan Sign Language is unwritable, just that it was often thought of as such by those who do not study sign languages.)

Generally, the influence literacy has on the status of a language is also addressed in debates of the so-called "written language paradigm" in which it is acknowledged that the availability of written language to some extent must be considered as a culturally and historically dependent phenomenon. Tim Ingold, a British anthropologist, discussed these matters at some length in Perception of the Environment (2000), though he does not specifically deal with ISN. Since 1996, however, Nicaraguans have been writing their language by hand and on computer using SignWriting. There are now many texts written in Nicaraguan Sign Language, including three volumes of reading lessons in ISN, Spanish I and II (two levels of texts, workbooks and primers), Cuentos Españoles (a collection of stories in Spanish with ISN glossaries), and a geography text.

See also

References

  1. ^ Nicaraguan Sign Language at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
  2. ^ Polich, L. (2005). The Emergence of the Deaf Community in Nicaragua: “With Sign Language You Can Learn So Much". Gallaudet University Press. https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctv2rcnfjw
  3. ^ Meir, Irit; Sandler, Wendy; Padden, Carol; Aronoff, Mark (2010). "Chapter 18: Emerging sign languages" (PDF). In Marschark, Marc; Spencer, Patricia Elizabeth (eds.). Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education. Vol. 2. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-539003-2. OCLC 779907637. Retrieved 2016-11-05.
  4. ^ Bouchard, Kelly (March 19, 2018). "Seeing the signs: Renowned USM professor reflects on life-changing language discovery". Press Herald. Retrieved October 13, 2018.
  5. ^ Ackerman, Felicia (November 28, 1999). "A Linguistic Big Bang". The New York Times. Retrieved October 13, 2018.
  6. ^ Nicaraguan Sign Language Projects, Inc. 2014.
  7. ^ Kegl, J. 2002. "Language Emergence in a Language-Ready Brain: Acquisition Issues". In Morgan, G. and Woll, B., Language Acquisition in Signed Languages. Cambridge University Press, pp. 207–254.
  8. ^ Senghas, R. J., Senghas, A., Pyers, J. E. 2005. "The emergence of Nicaraguan Sign Language: Questions of development, acquisition, and evolution". In Parker, S. T., Langer, J., and Milbrath, C. (eds.), Biology and Knowledge revisited: From neurogenesis to psychogenesis. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 287–306.
  9. ^ Bierma, Nathan. "At random on language: Nicaraguan deaf children create language of their own". The Chicago Tribune, October 14, 2004.
  10. ^ Senghas, Ann, and Marie Coppola. Children Creating Language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language Acquired a Spatial Grammar. Sage Publications. SAGE, July 1, 2001. p. 324.
  11. ^ Senghas, Ann, and Coppola, Marie. Children Creating Language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language Acquired a Spatial Grammar. Sage Publications. SAGE, July 1, 2001. p. 327.
  12. ^ Senghas, Ann. "The Emergence of Two Functions for Spatial Devices in Nicaraguan Sign Language". Karger.com. Karger, 2010. p. 295.
  13. ^ Senghas, Ann. Intergenerational influence and ontogenetic development in the emergence of spatial grammar in Nicaraguan Sign Language. Department of Psychology, Barnard College of Columbia University. Elsevier, 2003. p. 517.
  14. ^ Stokoe, William. "Letter to the Editor, Magazine". Retrieved February 25, 2013.
  15. ^ Tomasello, M. (2014). A Natural History of Human Thinking. MIT Press.

Further reading

External links

  • v
  • t
  • e
Official language
  • flagNicaragua portal
Indigenous languages
  • Garifuna
  • Miskito
  • Rama
  • Sumo
  • Creole languages
    Sign languages
    • Nicaraguan Sign Language
    • v
    • t
    • e
    Language
    families[a]
    Sign languages by family
    Australian
    Aboriginal
    (multiple families)[c]
    Western Desert
    Zendath Kesign
    Arab (Ishaaric)
    Iraqi–
    Levantine
    Levantine
    • Jordanian
    • Lebanese
    • Palestinian
    • Syrian
    Possible
    BANZSL
    Swedish Sign
    Chinese Sign
    Chilean-Paraguayan-
    Uruguayan Sign
    Paraguayan-
    Uruguayan Sign
    Francosign
    American
    (ASLic)
    Indonesian (Nusantaric)
    Francophone African
    (Françafrosign)
    • Ethiopian
    • Chadian
    • Ghanaian
    • Guinean
    • Bamako (LaSiMa)
    • Moroccan
    • Nigerian
    • Sierra Leonean
    Mixed, Hand Talk
    • Oneida (OSL)
    Mixed, Hoailona ʻŌlelo
    • Creole Hawaiʻi Sign Language (CHSL)
    Mixed, French (LSF)
    Austro-
    Hungarian
    Russian Sign
    Yugoslavic Sign
    Dutch Sign
    Italian Sign
    Mexican Sign
    Old Belgian
    Danish (Tegnic)
    Viet-Thai
    German Sign
    Indo-Pakistani
    Sign
    • Bangalore-Madras
    • Beluchistan
    • Bengali
    • Bombay
    • Calcutta
    • Delhi
    • Nepali
    • North West Frontier Province
    • Punjab-Sindh
    Japanese Sign
    Kentish[c]
    Mayan (Meemul Tziij)
    • Highland Maya
    • Yucatec
      • Chicán
      • Nohkop
      • Nohya
      • Trascorral
      • Cepeda Peraza
    Original Thai Sign
    Paget Gorman
    Plains Sign Language
    • Anishinaabe
    • Apsáalooke
    • Arikara
    • Chaticks si Chaticks
    • Cheyenne
    • Coahuilteco
    • Dane-zaa
    • Diné
    • Hinono'eino
    • Hiraacá
    • Icāk
    • Karankawa
    • Liksiyu
    • Maagiadawa
    • Meciciya ka pekiskwakehk
    • Nakota
    • Ni Mii Puu
    • Niimíipuu
    • Niitsítapi
    • Nųmą́khų́·ki
    • Nʉmʉnʉʉ
    • Omaha
    • Palus
    • Piipaash
    • Ppáⁿkka
    • Schitsu'umsh
    • Shiwinna
    • Sioux
    • Taos
    • Tickanwa•tic
    • Tháumgá
    • Tsuu T'ina
    • Umatilla
    • Wazhazhe
    • Wichita
    • Wíyut'a / Wíblut'e
    • Wyandot
    Mixed, American (ASL)
    • Oneida (OSL)
    Plateau
    • A'aninin
    • Kalispel
    • Ktunaxa (ʾa·qanⱡiⱡⱡitnam)
    • Nesilextcl'n
    • Shuswap (Secwepemcékst)
    • Sqeliz
    Providencia–
    Cayman Sign
    Isolates
    Other groupings
    By region[a]
    Sign languages by region
    Africa
    Algeria
    Algerian
    Ghardaia
    Cameroon
    Maroua
    Ghana
    Adamorobe (AdaSL / Mumu kasa)
    Nanabin
    Ivory Coast
    Bouakako (LaSiBo)
    Kenya
    Kenyan
    Mali
    Tebul
    Bamako (LaSiMa)
    Nigeria
    Bura
    Hausa (Magannar Hannu)
    Senegal
    Mbour
    Somalia, Somaliland & Djibouti
    Somali
    South Africa
    South African
    Tanzania
    Tanzanian
    Uganda
    Ugandan
    Zambia
    Zambian
    Asia
    Europe
    Armenia
    Armenian
    Austria
    Austrian
    Azerbaijan
    Azerbaijani
    Belgium
    Flemish
    French Belgian
    United Kingdom
    British
    Croatia
    Croatian
    Denmark
    Danish
    Faroese (Teknmál)
    Estonia
    Estonian
    Finland
    Finnish
    France
    Ghardaia
    French
    Lyons
    Germany
    German
    Greece
    Greek
    Hungary
    Hungarian
    Iceland
    Icelandic
    Ireland
    Irish
    Italy
    Italian
    Kosovo
    Yugoslav (Kosovar)
    Latvia
    Latvian
    Lithuania
    Lithuanian
    Moldova
    Russian
    Netherlands
    Dutch
    North Macedonia
    Macedonian
    Northern Ireland
    Northern Ireland
    Norway
    Norwegian
    Poland
    Polish
    Portugal
    Portuguese
    Russia
    Russian
    Slovenia
    Slovenian
    Spain
    Catalan
    Spanish
    Valencian
    Sweden
    Swedish
    Switzerland
    Swiss-German
    Turkey
    Central Taurus
    Mardin
    Turkish
    Ukraine
    Ukrainian
    North and
    Central
    America
    Plains Sign Talk
    Belize
    Belizean
    Canada
    American (ASL)
    Black ASL
    Protactile
    Blackfoot
    Cree
    Ojibwa
    Oneida
    Maritime (MSL)
    Quebec
    Inuit (Atgangmuurniq)
    Plateau
    Cayman
    Old Cayman
    Costa Rica
    Bribri
    Brunca
    Old Costa Rican
    New Costa Rican
    Cuba
    Cuban
    Greenland
    Greenlandic (Ussersuataarneq)
    Guatemala
    Guatemalan
    Mayan
    Haiti
    Haitian
    Honduras
    Honduran
    Mexico
    Albarradas
    Chatino
    Mayan
    Mexican
    Nicaragua
    Nicaraguan
    Panama
    Chiriqui
    Panamanian
    El Salvador
    Salvadoran
    Old Salvadoran
    United States
    American (ASL)
    Black ASL
    Protactile
    Blackfeet
    Cree
    Cheyenne
    Ojibwa
    Oneida
    Keresan (Keresign)
    Martha's Vineyard
    Navajo
    Navajo Family
    Sandy River Valley
    Henniker
    Oceania
    South America
    Argentina
    Argentine (LSA)
    Bolivia
    Bolivian
    Brazil
    Brazilian (Libras)
    Cena
    Ka'apor
    Chile
    Chilean
    Colombia
    Colombian
    Provisle
    Ecuador
    Ecuadorian
    Paraguay
    Paraguayan
    Peru
    Inmaculada
    Peruvian
    Sivia
    Uruguay
    Uruguayan
    Venezuela
    Venezuelan
    International
    ASL
    Extinct
    languages
    Linguistics
    Fingerspelling
    Writing
    Language
    contact
    Signed Oral
    Languages
    Others
    Media
    • Films (list)
    • Television shows (list)
    Persons
    Organisations
    Miscellaneous
    ^a Sign-language names reflect the region of origin. Natural sign languages are not related to the spoken language used in the same region. For example, French Sign Language originated in France, but is not related to French. Conversely, ASL and BSL both originated in English-speaking countries but are not related to each other; ASL however is related to French Sign Language.

    ^b Denotes the number (if known) of languages within the family. No further information is given on these languages.

    ^c Italics indicate extinct languages.